New perspectives in human behavior and culture


Man fixing nets

Samal man fixing nets for fishing in Mindanao, Philippines. Photo by Kim Hill.

|

It is at the confluence of different experiences that new theories come into being. Writing in this week’s “Perspectives” in the journal Science, Arizona State University researchers Kim Hill and Rob Boyd comment on new science analyzing human behavior in traditional societies and advocate for a new fully integrated evolutionary theory of human behavior.

A collaboration of these two particular researchers is not unexpected but reflects how the practical and theoretical combine to create new ideas. Hill has spent most of the last 30 years in the jungles of South and Central America, South Africa and the Philippines, living and working with Indigenous hunter-gatherer communities to understand the unique aspects of our own species. Boyd is a forerunner in the field of cultural evolution, focusing on the evolutionary psychology of the mechanisms that give rise to — and influence — human culture, and how these mechanisms interact with population dynamic processes to shape human cultural variation.

They are two of 17 scientists with the Institute of Human Origins who work on the cutting edge of evolutionary science to provide a better understanding of “how humans became human” and how and why we are in some ways so different from all other life forms on the planet. Both researchers are professors with the School of Human Evolution and Social Change.

In their commentary, Hill and Boyd support an analysis of 339 hunter-gatherer societies that shows that “not only are hunter-gatherers behaviorally similar in similar ecologies, but even mammals and birds in those ecologies tend to exhibit the same behavioral regularities as do human populations,” validating the evolutionary perspective called “human behavioral ecology.” 

However, shifting to a new paradigm that began in the 1980s, they add that social learning, cultural history, and cultural evolution are also important prime determinants of human behavioral variation. Humans cooperate more than any other primate. Because of the role of cultural cooperation, our species has seen spectacular ecological success.

Hill and Boyd also cite Institute of Human Origins researchers Sarah Mathew and Charles Perreault’s recent paper on the causes of variations among 172 North American Native American communities that found that “the effect of cultural history seems to persist for hundreds or even thousands of years.”

Together, Hill and Boyd see a need to synthesize both adaptive behavioral ecology and cultural evolution approaches into a singular, integrated evolutionary approach to understanding human behavioral variation.  

They state that “culture and genes are linked in a tight coevolutionary embrace, and this leads to complex patterns of genetic and cultural coadaptation.”

Hill and Boyd hope that these recent studies, their observations and new research currently being done will help elucidate the complex nature of human behavior and why explanations of human behavioral patterns will not simply be extensions of animal behavior models.

Commentary published in Science, "Behavioral convergence in humans and animals," by Kim Hill and Rob Boyd.

More Arts, humanities and education

 

Woman speaking into a microphone.

ASU alum's humanities background led to fulfilling job with the governor's office

As a student, Arizona State University alumna Sambo Dul was a triple major in Spanish, political science and economics. After…

Woman smiling and holding her arms out wide.

ASU English professor directs new Native play 'Antíkoni'

Over the last three years, Madeline Sayet toured the United States to tell her story in the autobiographical solo-…

A student looks through the book shelves in the Cross Cultural Dance Collection

ASU student finds connection to his family's history in dance archives

First-year graduate student Garrett Keeto was visiting the Cross-Cultural Dance Resources Collections at Arizona State University…