ASU study says our brains are flexible in choosing a partner, but we look to others for help


Illustrated image of two heads made up of puzzle pieces facing one another

Photo courtesy Getty Images Signature

|

As humans, we socially learn from others. We learn — or copy — how to talk from our parents and friends; we learn from teachers when we go to school.

And, according to science, people also tend to copy what others find attractive in partners. This mate-choice copying is said to save us time and energy when trying to find a match. 

Thomas Morgan
Thomas Morgan

“We probably all do this: We ask friends and family members for advice, discuss relationships with each other, and learn from our friends' successes and failures,” said Thomas Morgan, evolutionary anthropologist and associate professor at Arizona State University's School of Human Evolution and Social Change and research scientist at the Institute of Human Origins.

“Animals do it too. Female bowerbirds flock together and collectively inspect males; female fish watch where other females lay their eggs and follow suit. In many species, multiple females will mate with the same male. For humans, where most societies are broadly monogamous, this might constitute partner poaching, which probably would earn disapproval.”

Numerous studies about mate-choice copying have been conducted, and some scientists believe our brains evolved a context-specific system for picking a date. However, new research by Morgan shows different results. 

Morgan and former ASU graduate student Melanie Foreman ran a study that suggests the human brain is actually a flexible and adaptable broad-context social learning tool and not a collection of task-specific solutions programmed by evolution.

Morgan uses the metaphor of a Swiss-Army knife versus a crowbar to explain narrow and broad context copying mechanisms. 

This research encompasses the evolution of the human mind, evolutionary anthropology, psychology and biology fields Morgan has worked in over the span of his career. He is specifically interested in the evolution of the mind and intelligence. 

“Not only are humans cognitively sophisticated, but we have complex cultures, which themselves evolve and interact with genetic evolution too,” said Morgan. 

“When we talk about the evolution of the mind, we need to know what evolution has done exactly,” he said. “This gets to the heart of questions about human nature. Has natural selection given us a hardwired brain with dedicated circuits to solve a variety of common problems? Or has it given us a flexible, general-purpose learning system and then it’s up to us to figure things out?

"This is what this project helps us to understand, and its conclusion is the latter: Our brain is a flexible, general-purpose tool.”

Morgan and Foreman used the experimental platform Dallingerto to run the study with 164 people. Participants were separated into groups of 10 based on their primary romantic preference — either men, women or both. First the participants took a general knowledge quiz, then they were shown images of people’s faces and asked to rate the attractiveness of that person. 

After that, participants were shown the face images again, only this time they were able to see the quiz scores of other participants and they were able to see how other people in their group rated the images' attractiveness. 

Morgan wanted to gauge if participants would be influenced by conformist and prestige biases, both of which are known to be important when making other decisions. In other words: Would seeing others’ attractiveness ratings and seeing others’ test scores influence and change people's decisions? 

The answer: Yes.  

“We found that heterosexual men and women behaved very similarly when learning from others about hypothetical romantic partners, and they behaved similarly to when they made decisions of other kinds,” Morgan said. “This is consistent with the idea that the mind is a general-purpose learning tool. However, in many other species, there are differences. In particular, females typically put more effort into vetting and learning about potential partners than do males.

“Collectively, it looks like humans might be unique in the fact that we can learn from others about anything.”

The paper “Prestige, conformity and gender consistency support a broad-context mechanism underpinning mate-choice copying,” was published in the journal Evolution and Human Behavior. 

More Science and technology

 

A group of people wearing matching black jackets pose for a photo in front of ASU's Old Main building.

Indigenous geneticists build unprecedented research community at ASU

When Krystal Tsosie (Diné) was an undergraduate at Arizona State University, there were no Indigenous faculty she could look to in any science department. In 2022, after getting her PhD in genomics…

Collage of photos of covers of books by Professor Robert Boyd.

Pioneering professor of cultural evolution pens essays for leading academic journals

When Robert Boyd wrote his 1985 book “Culture and the Evolutionary Process,” cultural evolution was not considered a true scientific topic. But over the past half-century, human culture and cultural…

Man crouched in the dirt in a desert landscape.

Lucy's lasting legacy: Donald Johanson reflects on the discovery of a lifetime

Fifty years ago, in the dusty hills of Hadar, Ethiopia, a young paleoanthropologist, Donald Johanson, discovered what would become one of the most famous fossil skeletons of our lifetime — the 3.2…